Id |
Subject |
Object |
Predicate |
Lexical cue |
T282 |
0-4 |
Sentence |
denotes |
2.4. |
T283 |
5-23 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Structural Factors |
T284 |
25-31 |
Sentence |
denotes |
2.4.1. |
T285 |
32-44 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Social Class |
T286 |
45-231 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Uscinski and Parent [20] argue that conspiracy theories exist for “losers.” Thus, socially underprivileged individuals and members of the lower class tend to believe conspiracy theories. |
T287 |
232-426 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Mao et al. [61] show that social class can significantly negatively predict individuals’ beliefs in conspiracy theories and explain the likely link between social class and individuals’ beliefs. |
T288 |
427-616 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Because individuals from lower social classes receive less education, live in harsher environments, and often face external threats, they feel less control over their external environments. |
T289 |
617-726 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Generally, when perceived control is threatened by external factors, beliefs in conspiracy theories increase. |
T290 |
727-848 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Van Prooijen [62] finds that social class is correlated with the extent to which individuals believe conspiracy theories. |
T291 |
849-902 |
Sentence |
denotes |
A representative indicator of social class is income. |
T292 |
903-1022 |
Sentence |
denotes |
In empirical studies, Golec de Zavala and Federico [63] show that higher income reduces beliefs in conspiracy theories. |
T293 |
1023-1207 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Furthermore, Uscinski and Parent [20] demonstrate that people who work in the financial industry or for the government or the military exhibit the lowest levels of conspiracy thinking. |
T294 |
1208-1301 |
Sentence |
denotes |
However, the causal relationship between conspiracy beliefs and income is indeterminate [10]. |
T295 |
1302-1322 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Hypothesis 12 (H12). |
T296 |
1325-1461 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Belonging to a higher social class (i.e., having a higher income in this study) is negatively related to beliefs in conspiracy theories. |
T297 |
1463-1469 |
Sentence |
denotes |
2.4.2. |
T298 |
1470-1479 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Knowledge |
T299 |
1480-1554 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Knowledge plays critical roles in reducing beliefs in conspiracy theories. |
T300 |
1555-1688 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Miller et al. [3] show that those who are highly knowledgeable about politics tend to be the most susceptible to conspiracy theories. |
T301 |
1689-1889 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Moreover, this knowledge mediates the effect of ideology on the endorsement of conspiracy theories; knowledge increases ideologically motivated endorsements of conspiracy theories among conservatives. |
T302 |
1890-2046 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Moreover, Berinsky [31] shows that more knowledgeable people are less likely to believe in political conspiracies than their low-knowledge counterparts are. |
T303 |
2047-2187 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Goldberg and Richey [26] demonstrate that three different beliefs in conspiracy theories are positively correlated with political knowledge. |
T304 |
2188-2208 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Hypothesis 13 (H13). |
T305 |
2211-2275 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Knowledge has negative impact on beliefs in conspiracy theories. |
T306 |
2277-2283 |
Sentence |
denotes |
2.4.3. |
T307 |
2284-2293 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Education |
T308 |
2294-2374 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Many studies examine the relationship between education and conspiracy theories. |
T309 |
2375-2459 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Generally, lower education levels increase beliefs in conspiracy theories [7,61,64]. |
T310 |
2460-2609 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Van Prooijen [62] suggests that education may give people a set of cognitive and affective attributes that enable them to resist conspiracy theories. |
T311 |
2610-2820 |
Sentence |
denotes |
She describes the causal chain from education to beliefs in conspiracy theories as follows: less education → less analytic thinking → strong beliefs in simple solutions → greater beliefs in conspiracy theories. |
T312 |
2821-2965 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Also, Georgiou et al. [30] show that beliefs in conspiracy theories related to COVID-19 are greater among people with lower levels of education. |
T313 |
2966-2986 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Hypothesis 14 (H14). |
T314 |
2989-3060 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Education is negatively associated with beliefs in conspiracy theories. |
T315 |
3062-3068 |
Sentence |
denotes |
2.4.4. |
T316 |
3069-3080 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Information |
T317 |
3081-3142 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Information performs a function similar to that of knowledge. |
T318 |
3143-3238 |
Sentence |
denotes |
But both the quantity and quality of information are related to beliefs in conspiracy theories. |
T319 |
3239-3342 |
Sentence |
denotes |
In terms of the quantity, information related to a conspiracy theory reinforces beliefs in that theory. |
T320 |
3343-3489 |
Sentence |
denotes |
For example, Swami et al. [17] show that beliefs in conspiracies about 9/11 are positively related to exposure to the ideas of 9/11 conspiracists. |
T321 |
3490-3572 |
Sentence |
denotes |
In terms of quality, more elaborate information decreases beliefs in conspiracies. |
T322 |
3573-3665 |
Sentence |
denotes |
For example, more news media literacy is shown to reduce conspiracy theory endorsement [65]. |
T323 |
3666-3813 |
Sentence |
denotes |
It is not only the quantity and quality of information but also the style of information processing that influences beliefs in conspiracy theories. |
T324 |
3814-3912 |
Sentence |
denotes |
A conspiratorial mentality may partly reflect particular information-processing dispositions [29]. |
T325 |
3913-4063 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Thus, individuals’ engagement in seeking or finding meanings or patterns in ambiguous or random information may be related to conspiratorial thinking. |
T326 |
4064-4084 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Hypothesis 15 (H15). |
T327 |
4087-4194 |
Sentence |
denotes |
More qualitative and quantitative information is negatively associated with beliefs in conspiracy theories. |
T328 |
4196-4202 |
Sentence |
denotes |
2.4.5. |
T329 |
4203-4217 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Social Support |
T330 |
4218-4328 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Social structures that shape citizens’ feelings of vulnerability increase beliefs in conspiracy theories [66]. |
T331 |
4329-4432 |
Sentence |
denotes |
As a social structure, a social network or social support acts as a shield against conspiracy theories. |
T332 |
4433-4527 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Conversely, a lack of social resources or support promotes the beliefs in conspiracy theories. |
T333 |
4528-4631 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Freeman and Bentall [67] show that conspiracy believers are more likely to have weaker social networks. |
T334 |
4632-4748 |
Sentence |
denotes |
According to Grohol [68], any societal alienation appears to be connected to greater beliefs in conspiracy theories. |
T335 |
4749-4930 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Those who suffer on the edge of society, because of their unemployment status, minor ethnicity, or even weak relationship status, report stronger beliefs in the conspiracy theories. |
T336 |
4931-5132 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Conversely, Sapountzis and Condor [69] find that conspiracy narratives are as likely to be used by people with large social networks as by those whose social interactions are generally more restricted. |
T337 |
5133-5153 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Hypothesis 16 (H16). |
T338 |
5156-5239 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Individuals with more social support exhibit weaker beliefs in conspiracy theories. |
T339 |
5241-5247 |
Sentence |
denotes |
2.4.6. |
T340 |
5248-5261 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Health Status |
T341 |
5262-5388 |
Sentence |
denotes |
In the movie Conspiracy Theory, the main character, portrayed by Mel Gibson, is a patient who believes in a conspiracy theory. |
T342 |
5389-5479 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Even in the real world, health has a significant impact on beliefs in conspiracy theories. |
T343 |
5480-5687 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Barron et al. [70] note that the trait of schizotypy, characterized by perceptual, cognitive, and affective abnormalities, has been found to be a strong, positive predictor of beliefs in conspiracy theories. |
T344 |
5688-5969 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Coltheart [71] suggests that false beliefs occur because of neuropsychological impairments that (1) enforce the formation of such beliefs owing to faulty sensory information and (2) make it difficult to reject such beliefs owing to faulty prefrontal systems that evaluate thoughts. |
T345 |
5970-6198 |
Sentence |
denotes |
In addition, March and Springer [72] use a regression model to show that apart from odd beliefs or magical thinking and Machiavellianism, primary psychopathy is a significant positive predictor of beliefs in conspiracy theories. |
T346 |
6199-6377 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Conversely, Chen et al. [73] provide evidence that beliefs in conspiracy theories regarding the COVID-19 virus can predict the mental health and well-being of healthcare workers. |
T347 |
6378-6474 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Good health creates positive emotions and, consequently, weakens beliefs in conspiracy theories. |
T348 |
6475-6525 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Thus, the following hypothesis can be established. |
T349 |
6526-6546 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Hypothesis 17 (H17). |
T350 |
6549-6631 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Better health status is negatively associated with beliefs in conspiracy theories. |
T351 |
6633-6639 |
Sentence |
denotes |
2.4.7. |
T352 |
6640-6650 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Age/Gender |
T353 |
6651-6762 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Among demographic variables, age and gender have been examined as predictors of beliefs in conspiracy theories. |
T354 |
6763-6893 |
Sentence |
denotes |
According to Radnitz et al. [49] and Swami [25], young people are more likely to believe in conspiracy theories than older people. |
T355 |
6894-7012 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Moreover, in Goreis and Voracek’s [74] metastudies, youth is a positive predictor of a beliefs in conspiracy theories. |
T356 |
7013-7098 |
Sentence |
denotes |
However, Earnshaw et al. [2] find no significant impact of age on conspiracy beliefs. |
T357 |
7099-7212 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Regarding gender, Radnitz et al. [47] show that men have stronger beliefs in conspiracy theories than women have. |
T358 |
7213-7297 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Hart and Graether [29] show that conspiracy believers tend to be younger and female. |
T359 |
7298-7494 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Cassese et al. [75] demonstrate that men believe conspiracy theories more than women do because the former have higher levels of personal uncertainty and learned hopelessness than the latter have. |
T360 |
7495-7639 |
Sentence |
denotes |
In contrast, however, March and Springer [72] find that gender and age do not statistically significantly affect beliefs in conspiracy theories. |
T361 |
7640-7660 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Hypothesis 18 (H18). |
T362 |
7663-7747 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Younger people exhibit stronger beliefs in conspiracy theories than older people do. |
T363 |
7748-7768 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Hypothesis 19 (H19). |
T364 |
7771-7837 |
Sentence |
denotes |
Women exhibit stronger beliefs in conspiracy theories than men do. |